Comprehensive Physiology Wiley Online Library

Neuroscience and Higher Brain Function: From Myth to Public Responsibility

Full Article on Wiley Online Library



Abstract

The sections in this article are:

1 Prescientific Beginnings: Brain Succeeds Heart as Organ of Mind; Dualism Arises and Prevails
2 Early Neuroscience and Associated Speculations About Higher Brain Function
3 Antilocalizationist: Unitary View of Cerebral Function
4 Rise of Clinical Neuroscience and its Movement Toward Mental Materialism
5 Growth of Neuroanatomy Affirms a Materialist Position
6 Experimental Approaches Linking Clinical and Anatomic Studies to Neurophysiology
7 Jackson: Leading Clinical Neuroscientist of Nineteenth Century
8 Beginnings of Modern Evidence for Material Mind
9 The Present: Acceptance of Material Basis of Mind and Resulting Philosophical‐Social Effects
10 Future Goals of Neuroscience of Higher Brain Function
Figure 1. Figure 1.

Glyphs from Edwin Smith Papyrus depicting observations of an ancient physician or physicians. Arrow, case 8.

From Breasted 17.
Figure 2. Figure 2.

Enduring concepts in neurobiology. Leonardo da Vinci's drawing (ca. A.D. 1400) depicts a tripartite ventricular system (upper left). Reisch's wood cut (from 16th century manuscript) describes a tripartite mind almost certainly influenced by Plato's concept (upper right). MacLean's cartoon (20th century) of the “triune” brain reflects his thoughtful conclusions about limbic system (bottom) 149.

Figure 3. Figure 3.

Giotto's pre‐Renaissance (early 14th century), tradition‐breaking paintings showing human behavior reflecting vulgar emotion. Left: anger; right: malicious envy.

Figure 4. Figure 4.

Progress in neuroanatomy ranging from Leonardo da Vinci (15th century) (left) to Gall (19th century) (right).

Figure 5. Figure 5.

Cerebral localization from Gall and Spurzheim 59 to some of Gall's enthusiastic disciples. Left: from Gall and Spurzheim's 1817 atlas with numerical identification of prominent anatomic landmarks. Right: symbolic head depicting putative functional topography of brain, as reflected by palpable changes in the overlying skull.

Left from Gall and Spurzheim 59; right from Wells 144.
Figure 6. Figure 6.

Leborgne's brain described by Broca 19, later illustrated directly (left) and diagrammatically (right) by Marie 101. In addition to area described by Broca in third inferior frontal convolution, ischemic softening extends back along anterior superior temporal gyrus to affect underlying cortex, including lower part of supramarginal gyrus. Wernicke 145 designated the latter region as critical for language formation.

Figure 7. Figure 7.

Individual values for2‐fluorodeoxyglucose uptake measured by positron emission tomography in 1 locked‐in and 4 clinically vegetative patients compared with 18 subjects with normal hemispheric functions. Note that locked‐in patient, who had a large lesion in basis pontis, has reduced pontine uptake but normal rates of metabolism elsewhere. By contrast, uptake in vegetative brains is uniformly reduced in all areas by half or more compared with normal. [Data from Levy et al. 91.]

18F
Figure 8. Figure 8.

Positron emission tomography scans of2‐fluorodeoxyglucose uptake in single normal (upper left), locked‐in (upper right), and vegetative state patients (lower left). Color on scale at right indicates degree of glucose uptake: red, high; blue, low. Horizontal section was taken through hemispheres at level where temporal and occipital horns of ventricles converge, as reflected by low‐uptake parieto‐occipital areas in both scans. (Frontal lobes are toward top.) [Data from Levy et al. 91.]

18F


Figure 1.

Glyphs from Edwin Smith Papyrus depicting observations of an ancient physician or physicians. Arrow, case 8.

From Breasted 17.


Figure 2.

Enduring concepts in neurobiology. Leonardo da Vinci's drawing (ca. A.D. 1400) depicts a tripartite ventricular system (upper left). Reisch's wood cut (from 16th century manuscript) describes a tripartite mind almost certainly influenced by Plato's concept (upper right). MacLean's cartoon (20th century) of the “triune” brain reflects his thoughtful conclusions about limbic system (bottom) 149.



Figure 3.

Giotto's pre‐Renaissance (early 14th century), tradition‐breaking paintings showing human behavior reflecting vulgar emotion. Left: anger; right: malicious envy.



Figure 4.

Progress in neuroanatomy ranging from Leonardo da Vinci (15th century) (left) to Gall (19th century) (right).



Figure 5.

Cerebral localization from Gall and Spurzheim 59 to some of Gall's enthusiastic disciples. Left: from Gall and Spurzheim's 1817 atlas with numerical identification of prominent anatomic landmarks. Right: symbolic head depicting putative functional topography of brain, as reflected by palpable changes in the overlying skull.

Left from Gall and Spurzheim 59; right from Wells 144.


Figure 6.

Leborgne's brain described by Broca 19, later illustrated directly (left) and diagrammatically (right) by Marie 101. In addition to area described by Broca in third inferior frontal convolution, ischemic softening extends back along anterior superior temporal gyrus to affect underlying cortex, including lower part of supramarginal gyrus. Wernicke 145 designated the latter region as critical for language formation.



Figure 7.

Individual values for2‐fluorodeoxyglucose uptake measured by positron emission tomography in 1 locked‐in and 4 clinically vegetative patients compared with 18 subjects with normal hemispheric functions. Note that locked‐in patient, who had a large lesion in basis pontis, has reduced pontine uptake but normal rates of metabolism elsewhere. By contrast, uptake in vegetative brains is uniformly reduced in all areas by half or more compared with normal. [Data from Levy et al. 91.]

18F


Figure 8.

Positron emission tomography scans of2‐fluorodeoxyglucose uptake in single normal (upper left), locked‐in (upper right), and vegetative state patients (lower left). Color on scale at right indicates degree of glucose uptake: red, high; blue, low. Horizontal section was taken through hemispheres at level where temporal and occipital horns of ventricles converge, as reflected by low‐uptake parieto‐occipital areas in both scans. (Frontal lobes are toward top.) [Data from Levy et al. 91.]

18F
References
 1. Adrian, E. D. The Physical Background of Perception. London: Oxford Univ. Press, 1967. (Reprint. Oxford, UK: Clarendon, 1946.)
 2. Arts, W. F. M., H. R. Van Dongen, J. Van Hof‐Van Duin, and E. Lammens. Unexpected improvement after prolonged posttraumatic vegetative state. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 48: 1300–1303, 1985.
 3. Auburtin, E. Considération sur les localizations cérébrales, et en particulier sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé. Gaz. Hebd. Med. Chir. 10: 318–321, 348–351, 397–402, 455–458, 1863.
 4. Bastian, H. C. On the various forms of loss of speech in cerebral disease. Br. Med. Chir. Rev. 43: 209–236, 470–492, 1863.
 5. Bastian, H. C. Aphasia and Other Speech Disorders. New York: Appleton, 1898.
 6. Beecher, H. K. A definition of irreversible coma: report of the Ad Hoc Committee of the Harvard Medical School to examine the definition of brain death. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 205: 85–88, 1968.
 7. Betz, V. A. Anatomischer Nachweis zweier Gehirncentva. Centralbl. Med. Wiss. 12: 578–580, 595–599, 1874.
 8. Bevan‐Lewis, W. On the comparative structure of the cortex cerebri. Brain 1: 79–96, 1878.
 9. Bloom, F. E. (editor). Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Intrinsic Regulatory Systems of the Brain. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1986, vol. IV.
 10. Bonin, G. von.. Some Papers on the Cerebral Cortex. Spring‐field, IL: Thomas, 1960. (Transl. from French and German.)
 11. Borst, C. V. The Mind‐Brain Identity Theory. New York: St. Martin, 1970.
 12. Bouillaud, J. Recherches cliniques propres à démonstrer que la perte de la parole correspond à la lésion des lobules antérieurs du cerveau, et à confirmer l'opinion de M. Gall sur le siège de l'organe du langage articulé. Arch. Gen. Med. 8: 25–45, 1825.
 13. Brain and Mind. New York: Excerpta Med., 1979. (Ciba Found. Symp. 69.)
 14. Brazier, M. A. B. The historical development of neurophysiology. In: Neurophysiology, edited by J. Field and H. W. Magoun. Washington, DC: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1959, vol. I, chapt. 1, p. 1–58.
 15. Brazier, M. A. B. The growth of concepts relating to brain mechanisms. J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 1: 218–234, 1965.
 16. Brazier, M. A. B. A History of Neurophysiology in the 17th and 18th Century. New York: Raven, 1984.
 17. Breasted, J. H. The Edwin Smith Surgical Papyrus. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press, 1930, vols. I and II, p. 201–216.
 18. Brett, G. S. History of Psychology, edited by R. S. Peters. London: Allen & Unwin, 1962.
 19. Broca, P. Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé: suivies d'une observation d'aphémie. Bull. Soc. Anat. Paris 36: 330–357, 1861.
 20. Brodmann, K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Groshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig, Germany: Barth, 1909.
 21. Brooks, V. B. (editor). Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Motor Control. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1981, vol. II.
 22. Bunge, M. The Mind‐Body Problem. New York: Pergamon, 1980.
 23. Campbell, A. W. Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral Function. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1905.
 24. Clarke, E., and C. D. O'Malley. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord. Berkeley: Univ. of California Press, 1968.
 25. Cushing, H. The Pituitary Body and Hypothalamus. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1929.
 26. Darian‐Smith, I. (editor). Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Sensory Processes. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1984, vol. III.
 27. Darwin, C. Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. New York: Appleton, 1899.
 28. Darwin, C. The Autobiography of Charles Darwin and Selected Letters. New York: Dover, 1958.
 29. Darwin, C. The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection. New York: Macmillan, 1962.
 30. Darwin, C. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press, 1981.
 31. Dax, M. Lésions de la moitié gauche de l'encéphale coincidant avec l'oublie des signes de la pensée. Montpellier Méd. 27: 236–345, 1877.
 32. Debus, A. G. Man and Nature in the Renaissance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1978.
 33. Deiters, O. F. K. Untersuchungen über Gehirn und Rückenmark des Menschen und der Säugethiere. Braunschweig, Germany: F. Vieweg, 1865.
 34. Descartes, R. Discours de la méthode pour bien conduire sa raison. Leiden, The Netherlands: Maire, 1637. [Reprinted in Descartes (35).]
 35. Descartes, R. Ouevres de Descartes Publiées par C. Adam et P. Tannery. Paris: Vrin, 1956–1957, 13 vols.
 36. Donnelly, M. Managing the Mind. London: Tavistock, 1983.
 37. Dougherty, J. H., D. E. Rawlinson, D. E. Levy, and F. Plum. Hypoxic ischemic brain injury and the vegetative state. Clinical and neurological correlation. Neurology 31: 991–999, 1981.
 38. Edelman, G., and V. Mountcastle. The Mindful Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1978.
 39. Englehardt, H. T. John Hughlings Jackson and the mind‐body relation. Bull. Hist. Med. 49: 137–151, 1975.
 40. Exner, S. Untersuchungen über Die Localisation der Funktionen in der Grosshirnrinde des Menschen. Vienna: Braumüller, 1881.
 41. Feigl, H. The “mental” and the “physical” In: Concepts, Theories and the Mind‐Body Problem, edited by H. Feigl, M. Scriven, and G. Maxwell. Minneapolis: Univ. of Minnesota Press, 1958, vol. II, p. 370–497. (Minn. Stud. Philos. Sci.)
 42. Feigl, H. Mind‐body, not a pseudo‐problem. In: The Mind‐Brain Identity Theory, edited by C. V. Borst. New York: St. Martin, 1970, p. 33–41.
 43. Ferrier, D. The localization of function in the brain (Abstract). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 22: 229–232, 1874.
 44. Ferrier, D. Experiments on the brains of monkeys. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. 165: 433–488, 1876.
 45. Ferrier, D. The Functions of the Brain. Croonian Lectures 1874–75. London: Smith & Elder, 1876.
 46. Ferrier, D. The Localization of Cerebral Disease. London: Smith & Elder, 1878.
 47. Field, J., and H. W. Magoun (editors). Handbook of Physiology. Neurophysiology. Washington, DC: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1959–1960, vols. I–III.
 48. Flanagan, O. J. The Science of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1984.
 49. Flechsig, P. Developmental localisation of the cerebral cortex in the human subject. Lancet 10: 1027–1029, 1901.
 50. Flourens, P. Recherches experimentales sur les propriétés et les fonctions du système nerveux dans les animaux vertébrés. Paris: Crevot, 1824, p. 85–122.
 51. Flourens, P. Phrenology Examined, translated by C. Meigs. Philadelphia, PA: Hogan & Thompson, 1846, p. 21–28.
 52. Fodor, J. A. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1983.
 53. Foerster, O. The motor cortex in man in the light of Hughlings Jackson's doctrines. Brain 59: 135–159, 1936.
 54. Forel, A. H. Einige hirnanatomische Betrachtungen und Ergebnisse. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 18: 162–198, 1887.
 55. Freud, S. The Origins of Psychoanalysis. New York: Basic, 1954.
 56. Fritsch, G., and E. Hitzig. Uber die electrische Erregbarkeit des Grosshirns. Arch. Anat. Physiol. Wiss. 37: 330–332, 1870.
 57. Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex. New York: Raven, 1980.
 58. Galanter, E. Detection and discrimination of environmental change. In: Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Sensory Processes, edited by I. Darian‐Smith. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1984, vol. III, pt. 1, chapt. 3, p. 103–121.
 59. Gall, F. J., and G. Spurzheim. Anatomie et physiologie du système nerveux en general, et du cerveau en particulier, avec des observations sur la possibilité de reconnoître plusieures dispositions intellectuelles et morales de l'homme et des animaux par la configuration de leurs têtes. Paris: Schoell, 1810–1819, 4 vols.
 60. Garrison, W. E. The History of Neurology. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1958.
 61. Geschwind, N. Disconnection syndromes in animals and man. Brain 88: 237–294, 585–694, 1965.
 62. Gibson, W. C. Pioneers in localization of function in the brain. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 180: 944–951, 1962.
 63. Gilmore, M. P. The World of Humanism 1453–1517. New York: Harper & Row, 1952.
 64. Haymaker, W. E. Founders of Neurology. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1953.
 65. Hebb, D. O. Essay on Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980.
 66. Hécaen, H., and M. L. Alvot. Human Neuropsychology. New York: Levy, 1978.
 67. Helmholtz, H. von. Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik 1st Edition 1859–67, edited by J. P. C. Southall. Washington, DC: Opt. Soc. Am., 1924–1925. English transl. (Reprint. New York: Dover, 1962.)
 68. Higashi, K., M. Hatano, S. Abiko, K. Ihara, S. Katayama, Y. Wakuta, T. Okamura, and T. Yamashita. Five‐year follow‐up of patients with persistent vegetative state. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 44: 552–554, 1981.
 69. His, W. Die Entwickelung des Menschlichen Gehirns Während der Ersten Monate. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel, 1904.
 70. Hochberg, J. Perception. In: Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Sensory Processes, edited by I. Darian‐Smith. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1984, vol. III, pt. 1, chapt. 2, p. 75–102.
 71. Jackson, J. H. Clinical remarks on cases of defects of expression in diseases of the nervous system. Lancet 2: 604, 1864.
 72. Jackson, J. H. Croonian lectures on evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. Br. Med. J. 1: 591, 660, 703, 1884.
 73. Jackson, J. H. A contribution to the comparative study of convulsions. Brain 9: 1–23, 1886.
 74. Jackson, J. H. Lumlein lectures on convulsive seizures. Br. Med. J. 1: 703, 765, 821, 1890.
 75. Jackson, J. H. Papers on aphasia and kindred affections of speech. Brain 38: 1–190, 1915.
 76. Jackson, J. H. Selected Writings of John Hughlings Jackson, edited by J. Taylor. London: Hodder & Stoughton, 1931, 2 vols.
 77. Jefferson, G. The prodromes to cortical localization. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 16: 59–72, 1953.
 78. Jennett, B., G. Teasdale, R. Braakman, J. Minderhoud, J. Heiden, and T. Kurze. Prognosis of patients with severe head injury. Neurosurgery 4: 283–289, 1979.
 79. Jones, E. G., and T. P. S. Powell. Connexions of the somatic sensory cortex of the rhesus monkey. I. Ipsilateral cortical connexions. Brain 92: 477–502, 1969.
 80. Jones, E. G., and T. P. S. Powell. Connexions of the somatic sensory coretex of the rhesus monkey. II. Contralateral cortical connexions. Brain 92: 717–730, 1969.
 81. Jones, E. G., and T. P. S. Powell. Connexions of the somatic sensory cortex of the rhesus monkey. III. Thalamic connexions. Brain 93: 37–56, 1970.
 82. Jones, E. G., and T. P. S. Powell. An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain 93: 793–820, 1970.
 83. Jung, R. Sensory research in historical perspective: some philosophical foundations of perception. In: Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Sensory Processes, edited by I. Darian‐Smith. Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol. Soc., 1984, vol. III, pt. 1, chapt. 1, p. 1–74.
 84. Kety, S. S., and C. F. Schmidt. The nitrous oxide method for the quantitative determination of cerebral blood flow in man: theory, procedure and normal values. J. Clin. Invest. 27: 107–119, 1948.
 85. La Mettrie, J. O. de. L'Homme machine. Leiden, 1748. (Critical ed., with introductory monograph, transl. by A. Vartanian. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press, 1960.)
 86. Lassen, N. A., and D. H. Ingvar. Radioisotope assessment of regional cerebral blood flow. Proc. Nucl. Med. 1: 376–409, 1972.
 87. Levy, D. E., D. Bates, J. J. Coronna, N. E. F. Cartlidge, R. P. Knill‐Jones, R. H. Lapinski, B. H. Singer, D. A. Shaw, and F. Plum. Prognosis in non‐traumatic coma. Ann. Intern. Med. 94: 293–301, 1981.
 88. Levy, D. E., J. J. Coronna, B. H. Singer, R. H. Lapinski, H. Frydman, and F. Plum. Predicting outcome from hypoxic‐ischemic coma. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 253: 1420–1426, 1985.
 89. Levy, D. E., R. P. Knill‐Jones, and F. Plum. The vegetative state and its prognosis following non‐traumatic coma. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 315: 293–306, 1978.
 90. Levy, D. E., D. A. Rottenberg, J. O. Jarden, S. C. Strother, V. Dhawan, J. Z. Ginos, and F. Plum. PET studies of rCMRGlu in vegetative and locked‐in patients. Neurology 36, Suppl. 1: 282, 1986.
 91. Levy, D. E., J. J. Sidtis, D. A. Rottenberg, J. O. Jarden, S. C. Strother, V. J. Dhawan, J. Z. Ginos, and F. Plum. Positron emission tomographic (PET) measurement of regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose and blood flow in vegetative and locked‐in patients (Abstract). Ann. Neurol. 20: 122–123, 1986.
 92. Lichtheim, L. On aphasia. Brain 7: 433–460, 1885.
 93. Liepmann, H. K. Das Krankheitschild der Apraxie (motorischen Asymbolie) auf Grund eines Falles von einseitiger Apraxie. Monatsschr. Psychiatr. Neurol. 8: 15, 102, 182, 1900.
 94. Locke, J. Essay Concerning Human Understanding. London: Holt, 1690, 4 vols.
 95. Longuet, F. A. Anatomie et physiologie du système nerveux de l'homme et des animaux vertébrés. Paris: Masson, 1842.
 96. Lorenté de Nó, R. Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex. I. The area entorhinalis. J. Psychol. Neurol. 45: 381–438, 1934.
 97. Lorenté de Nó, R. Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex. II. Continuation of the study of the ammonic system. J. Psychol. Neurol. 46: 112–177, 1934.
 98. Lorenté de Nó, R. Analysis of the activity of the chains of internuncial neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1: 207–244, 1938.
 99. Luria, A. R. The problem of localization of function in the cerebral cortex. In: Higher Cortical Functions in Man, edited by A. R. Luria. New York: Basic, 1966, p. 5–38. (Transl. by B. Haigh.)
 100. MacLean, P. D. The brain, empathy and education. J. Nerve Ment. Dis. 144: 374, 1967.
 101. Magendie, P. Leçons sur les fonctions et les maladies du système nerveux. Paris: Ebrard, 1839.
 102. Marie, P. Essai de critique historique sur la genèse de la doctrine de Broca. Sem. Med. 26: 565–571, 1906.
 103. Matteucci, C. Sur l'existence du courant électrique dans les animaux vivants ou récemment tués. C. R. Acad. Sci. 16: 197, 1843.
 104. Meynert, T. Fall von Sprachstörung anatomisch begründet. Vienna: Med. Jährb, 1866.
 105. Meynert, T. Der bau der Grosshirnrinde und seine örtlichen Verschiedenheiten nebst einem pathologisch‐ánatomischen Corollarium. Vierteljahrsschr. Psychiatr. 1: 77–93, 198–217; 2: 88–113, 1867–1868.
 106. Mollaret, P., and M. Goulon. Le coma depassé. Rev. Neurol. 101: 3–15, 1959.
 107. Mountcastle, V. B. An organizing principle for cerebral function: the unit module and the distributed system. In: The Mindful Brain, edited by G. M. Edelman and V. B. Mountcastle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1978, p. 7–50.
 108. Munk, H. Uber die Funktionen der Grosshirnrinde. Third Mittheilung. Berlin: Hirschwald, 1880. [English transl. in Bonin (8), p. 97–117.]
 109. Newton, I. Philosophiae Naturalis Principia Mathematica (3rd ed.), edited by A. Koyré and I. B. Cohen. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1972, 2 vols.
 110. Onians, R. B. The Origins of European Thought. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1951.
 111. Penfield, W., and H. Jasper. Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain. Boston, MA: Little, Brown, 1954.
 112. Penfield, W., and T. Rasmussen. The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York: Macmillan, 1950.
 113. Plum, F., and J. B. Posner. The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma (3rd rev. ed.) Philadelphia, PA: Davis, 1982.
 114. Popper, K. R., and J. C. Eccles. The Self and Its Brain. New York: Springer, 1977.
 115. Purkinje, J. E. Uber die gangliösen Korperchen in verschiedenen Theilen des Gehirns. Bericht über die Versammlung deutscher Naturforscher und Aerzte. 15: 174, 1838.
 116. Ramón Y. Cajal, S. Structure et connexions des neurones. Arch. Fisiol. 5: 1–25, 1908.
 117. Ramón Y. Cajal, S. Considérations anatomiques et physiologiques sur le cerveau. In: Histologie du Système Nerveux de L'homme et des Vertébrés, traduite de l'espagnol par L. Azoulay. Paris: Maloine, 1909.
 118. Reese, H. The history of aphasia. Bull. Hist. Med. 21: 322–334, 1947.
 119. Reeves, J. W. Body and Mind in Western Thought. Middlesex, UK: Penguin, 1958.
 120. Roland, P. E. Organization of motor control by the normal human brain. Hum. Neurobiol. 2: 205–216, 1984.
 121. Roland, P. E., and L. Friberg. Localization of cortical areas activated by thinking. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 1219–1243, 1985.
 122. Romanes, G. J. Mental Evolution in Animals With a Posthumous Essay on Instinct by Charles Darwin. New York: Appleton, 1884.
 123. Rose, F. C., and W. F. Bynum. Historical Aspects of the Neurosciences. New York: Raven, 1984.
 124. Rosenberg, G. A., S. F. Johnson, and R. P. Brenner. Recovery of cognition after prolonged vegetative state. Ann. Neurol. 2: 167–168, 1977.
 125. Rowse, A. L. The Elizabethan Renaissance. New York: Scribner, 1972.
 126. Runes, D. D. Dictionary of Philosophy. New York: Philosophical Library, 1983.
 127. Schmitt, F. O., F. G. Worden, A. Adelman, and S. G. Dennis (editors). The Organization of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1981.
 128. Sheed, F. J. (translator). Saint Augustine Confessions. New York: Sheed & Ward, 1942, book 10, p. 8–25.
 129. Sigerist, H. E. A History of Medicine. New York: Oxford Univ. Press, 1951, 2 vols.
 130. Smith, C. U. M. Evolution and the problem of mind. Pt. II. John Hughlings Jackson. J. Hist. Biol. 15: 241–262, 1982.
 131. Snyder, B. D., R. E. Cranford, A. B. Rubens, S. Dundlie, and G. E. Rockswold. Delayed recovery from postanoxic persistent vegetative state (Abstract). Ann. Neurol. 14: 152A, 1983.
 132. Spencer, H. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Appleton, 1897.
 133. Sperry, R. W. A Modified Concept of Consciousness. Psychol. Rev. 76: 532–536, 1969.
 134. Stookey, B. A note on the early history of cerebral localization. Bull. NY Acad. Sci. 30: 559–578, 1954.
 135. Temkin, O. Gall and the phrenological movement. Bull. Hist. Med. 21: 275–321, 1947.
 136. Temkin, O. The Falling Sickness (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press, 1971.
 137. Teuber, H. L. Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 32: 615–656, 1971.
 138. Teuber, H. L. Effects of focal brain injury on human behavior. In: The Nervous System, edited by D. B. Tower. New York: Raven, 1975, vol. 2.
 139. Vogt, O. Über structurelle Hirnzentral, mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der strukturellen. Felder des cortex pallii. Anat. Anz. Verh. Anat. Ges. 29: 74–114, 1906.
 140. Vogt, O., and C. Vogt. Zur anatomischen Gliederung des Cortex cerebri. J. Psychol. Neurol. 2: 160–180, 1903.
 141. Von Monakow, C. Lokalisation der Hirnfunktionen. J. Neurol. Psychiatry 17: 185–200, 1911. [English transl. in Bonin (8), p. 231–250.]
 142. Walker, A. E. The development of the concept of cerebral localization in the 19th century. Bull. Hist. Med. 31: 99–121, 1957.
 143. Walker, A. E. Stimulation and ablation; their role in the history of cerebral physiology. J. Neurophysiol. 20: 435–449, 1957.
 144. Waller, A. V. Experiments on the section of the glossopharyngeal and hypoglossal nerves of the frog. Philos. Trans. 140: 423, 1850.
 145. Wells, R. D. B. Handbook of Phrenology. London: Vickers, ca. 1880.
 146. Wernicke, C. The symptom complex of aphasia. In: Diseases of the Nervous System, edited by A. Church. New York: Appleton, 1908, p. 265–324.
 147. Wikler, D. I. Conceptual issues in the definition of death: a guide for public policy. Theor. Med. 5: 167–180, 1984.
 148. Young, R. M. The functions of the brain from Gall to Ferrier (1808–1886). Isis 59: 251–258, 1968.
 149. Zülch, K. J. Critical remarks on “Lokalisationslehre”. In: Cerebral Localization, edited by K. J. Zülch, O. Creutzfeldt, and G. C. Galbraith. New York: Springer‐Verlag, 1975, p. 3–16.

Contact Editor

Submit a note to the editor about this article by filling in the form below.

* Required Field

How to Cite

Fred Plum, Bruce T. Volpe. Neuroscience and Higher Brain Function: From Myth to Public Responsibility. Compr Physiol 2011, Supplement 5: Handbook of Physiology, The Nervous System, Higher Functions of the Brain: 1-23. First published in print 1987. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp010501